References

Abe, M., Hanakawa, T., Takayama, Y., Kuroki, C., Ogawa, S., & Fukuyama, H. (2007) . Functional Coupling of Human Prefrontal and Premotor Areas during Cognitive Manipulation. Journal of Neuroscience27 (13) , 3429-3438.

Adcock, R. A., Constable, R. T., Gore, J. C., & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (2000). Functional neuroanatomy of executive processes involved in dual-task performance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97 (7), 3567-3572.

Aguirre, G. K., Zarahn, E., & D'Esposito, M . (1998). The variability of human BOLD hemodynamic responses. NeuroImage, 8 (4) , 360-9. 

Andersen, R.A., Essick, G.K., & Siegel, R.M. (1987 ). Neurons of area 7 are activated by both visual stimuli and oculomotor behavior. Experimental Brain Research, 67 (2)316-322.

Arnell, K. M., & Duncan, J. (2002). Separate and shared sources of dual-task cost in stimulus identification and response selection. Cognitive psychology, 44 (2), 105-47.

Aron, A.R., Monsell, S., Sahakian, B.J., & Robbins, T.W. (2004). A componential analysis of task-switching deficicts associated with lesions of the left and right frontal cortex. Brain, 127 (7) , 1561-1573.

Baddeley, A . (1998). Recent developments in working memory. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 8 (2), 234-238.

Badre, D., & Wagner, A. D. (2004). Selection, integration, and conflict monitoring; assessing the nature and generality of prefrontal cognitive control mechanisms. Neuron, 41 (3), 473-487.

Bandettini, P. A., Wong, E. C., Hinks, R. S., Tikofsky, R. S., & Hyde, J. S. (1992). Time course EPI of human brain function during task activation. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 25 (2), 390-397. 

Banich, M. T., Milham, M. P., Jacobson, B. L., Webb, A., Wszalek, T., Cohen, N. J., & Kramer, A.F. (2001). Attentional selection and the processing of task-irrelevant information: insights from fMRI examinations of the Stroop task . Progress in brain research, 134, 459-470.

Barbas, H., Ghashghaei, H.T., Rempel-Clower, N.L., & Xiao, D . (2002). Anatomic basis of functional specialization in prefrontal cortices in primates . In: Grafman, J., (ed .). Handbook of neuropsychology, 2nd edition, volume 7 . Amsterdam : Elsevier Science , p 1–27.

Barceló, F., & Knight, R.T. (2000). Both random and perseverative errors underlie WCST deficits in prefrontal patients. Neuropsychologia, 40 (3), 349-356.

Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108 (3), 624-652. 

Brass, M., Derrfuss, J., Forstmann, B., & von Cramon, D.Y. (2005). The role of the inferior frontal junction area in cognitive control . Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 9 (7), 314-316.

Braver, T.S., Cohen, J.D., Nystrom, L.E., Jonides, J., Smith, E.E., & Noll, D.C . (1997). A parametric study of prefrontal cortex involvement in human working memory. Neuroimage, 5 (1), 49-62.

Braver, T. S., Reynolds, J. R., & Donaldson, D. I. (2003). Neural mechanisms of transient and sustained cognitive control during task switching. Neuron, 39 (4), 713-726.

Brodmann, K. (1909). Vergleichende Lokalisationslehre der Grosshirnrinde. Prinzipien dargestellt auf der Grund des Zelllenbaus . Leipzig: Barth.

Bunge, S. A., Klingberg, T., Jacobsen, R. B., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2000). A resource model of the neural basis of executive working memory . Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97 (7), 3573-3578. 

Bunge, S.A., Kahn, I., Wallis, J.D., Miller, E.K., & Wagner, A.D. (2003 ). Neural circuits subserving the retrieval and maintenance of abstract rules . Journal of Neurophysiology, 903419-3428.

Burgess, P. W., Veitch, E., de Lacy Costello, A., & Shallice, T (2000). The cognitive and neuroanatomical correlates of multitasking . Neuropsychologia, 38 (6), 848-863.

Burgess, P. W., Scott, S. K., & Frith, C. D . (2003). The role of the rostral frontal cortex (area 10) in prospective memory: a lateral versus medial dissociation . Neuropsychologia, 41 (8), 906-918. 

Casey, B. J., Thomas, K. M., Welsh, T. F., Badgaiyan, R. D., Eccard, C. H., Jennings, J. R., & Crone, E.A . (2000). Dissociation of response conflict, attentional selection, and expectancy with functional magnetic resonance imaging . Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97 (15 ), 8728-33.

Chao, L.L., & Knight, R.T. (1995). Human prefrontal lesions increase distractability to irrelevant sensory inputs. Neuroreport, 6 (12), 1605-1610.

Christoff, K., & Gabrieli, J.D.E. (2000). The frontopolar cortex and human cognition: Evidence for a rostrocaudal hierarchical organization within the human prefrontal cortex. Psychobiology, 28 (2) , 168-186.

Collette, F., Olivier, L., Van der Linden, M., Laureys, S., Delfiore, G., Luxen, A., & Salmon, E. (2005). Involvement of both prefrontal and inferior parietal cortex in dual-task performance. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research , 24 (2 ), 237-251. 

Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3 (3), 201-215.

Culham, J.C., & Kanwisher, N.G. (2001 ). Neuroimaging of cognitive functions in human parietal cortex. Current Opinions in Neurobiology , 11 (2), 157-163.

de Fockert, J. W., Rees, G., Frith, C. D., & Lavie, N . (2001). The role of working memory in visual selective attention. Science , 291 (5509 ), 1803-1806. 

De Jong, R . (1995). The role of preparation in overlapping task performance. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psycholgy. 48 (1), 2-25.

D’Esposito, M., Detre, J.A, Alsop, D.C., Shin, R.K., Atlas, S., & Grossmann, M. (1995). The neural basis of the central executive system of working memory. Nature , 378279-281.

Desimone, R., & Duncan, J . (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience , 18, 193-222.

Di Pellegrino, G., & Wise, S.P. (1993 ). Visuospatial versus visuomotor activity in the premotor and prefrontal cortex of a primate . Journal of Neuroscience , 13 (3)1227-1243.

Dove, A., Pollmann, S., Schubert, T., Wiggins, C.J., & von Cramon, D.Y . (2000 ). Prefrontal cortex activation in task switching: an event-related fMRI study. Cognitive Brain Research, 9 (1), 103-109.

Duncan, J. (1979). Divided attention: The whole is more th a n the sum of its parts. Dual-task interference as an indicator of on-line programming in simple movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5 (2), 216-228.

Duncan, J. (2001). An adaptive coding model of neural function in prefrontal cortex . Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2 , 820-829.

Dux, P.E., Ivanoff, J., Aplund, C.L., & Marois, R . (2006). Isolation of a central bottleneck of information processing with time-resolved fMRI. Neuron , 52, 1109-1120.

Egner, T., & Hirsch, J. (2005). Cognitive control mechanisms resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task-relevant information . Nature Neuroscience, 8 (12), 1784-90. 

Erickson, K.I., Colcombe, S.J., Wadhwa, R., Bherer, L., Peterson, M.S., Scalf, P.E., & Kramer, A.F. (2005a). Neural correlates of dual-task performance after minimizing task-preparation . Neuroimage, 28 (4), 695-709.

Erickson, K. I., Ringo Ho, M., Colcombe, S. J., & Kramer, A. F. (2005b). A structural equation modeling analysis of attentional control: an event-related fMRI study. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research, 22 (3), 349-57.

Fan, J., Flombaum, J. I., McCandliss, B. D., Thomas, K. M., & Posner, M. I. (2003). Cognitive and brain consequences of conflict. NeuroImage , 18 (1), 42-57. 

Friston, K. J., Frith, C. D., Turner, R., & Frackowiak, R. S . (1995a). Characterizing evoked hemodynamics with fMRI . NeuroImage , 2 (2), 157-65.

Friston, K.J., Holmes, A.P., Worsley, K.J., Poline, J.-P., Frith, C.D., & Frackowiak, R.S.J. (1995b). Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach. Human Brain Mapping, 2 (4), 189-210.

Friston, K. J., Buechel, C., Fink, G. R., Morris, J., Rolls, E., & Dolan, R. J. (1997). Psychophysiological and modulatory interactions in neuroimaging. NeuroImage, 6 (3), 218-229.

Friston, K. J., Rotshtein, P., Geng, J. J., Sterzer, P., & Henson, R. N . (2006). A critique of functional localisers. NeuroImage, 30 (4), 1077-87.

Fuster, J.M. (1989): The Prefrontal Cortex . New York: Raven Press.

Fuster, J.M. (2001). The Prefrontal Cortex – An Update: Time is of the essence. Neuron, 30, 319-333.

Fuster, J.M, & Alexander, G.E. (1971). Neuron activity related to short-term memory . Science, 173 (997)652-654.

Gazzaley, A., Rissman, J., & Desposito, M . (2004). Functional connectivity during working memory maintenance. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 4 (4), 580-599.

Goldberg, T. E., Berman, K. F., Fleming, K., Ostrem, J., Van Horn, J. D., Esposito, G., Mattay, V.S., Gold, J.M., & Weinberger, D.R.. (1998). Uncoupling cognitive workload and prefrontal cortical physiology: a PET rCBF study. NeuroImage, 7 (4.1), 296-303.

Goldman-Rakic, P.S. (1987 ). Circuitry of primate prefrontal cortex and regulation of behaviour by representational memory. In F. Plum (Ed.), Handbook of physiology - The nervous system V , 373-417. New York: Oxford University Press.

Grant, D.A. & Berg, E.A . (1948). A behavioral analysis of degree of in reinforcement and ease of shifting to new responses in Weigl-type card-sorting problem. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 404-411.

Handwerker, D. A., Ollinger, J. M., & D'Esposito, M . (2004). Variation of BOLD hemodynamic responses across subjects and brain regions and their effects on statistical analyses . NeuroImage, 21 (4), 1639-51.

Hazeltine, E., Poldrack, R., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2000). Neural activation during response competition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12 Suppl 2, 118-129.

Hazeltine, E., Ruthruff, E., & Remington, R. W. (2006). The role of input and output modality pairings in dual-task performance: evidence for content-dependent central interference. Cognitive Psychology , 52 (4), 291-345.

Hein, G., & Schubert, T. (2004). Aging and input processing in dual-task situations. Psychology and Aging, 19 (3), 416-32. 

Herath, P., Klingberg, T., Young, J., Amunts, K., & Roland, P . (2001 ). Neural correlates of dual task interference can be dissociated from those of divided attention: an fMRI study. Cerebral Cortex, 11 (9)796-805.

Hester, R., Murphy, K., & Garavan, H. (2004 ). Beyond common resources: the cortical basis for resolving task interference. NeuroImage, 23 (1), 202-12.

Hopfinger, J. B., Buonocore, M. H., & Mangun, G. R. (2000). The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control. Nature neuroscience , 3 (3), 284-91. 

Jaeggi, S. M., Seewer, R., Nirkko, A. C., Eckstein, D., Schroth, G., Groner, R., & Gutbrod, K. (2003). Does excessive memory load attenuate activation in the prefrontal cortex? Load-dependent processing in single and dual tasks: functional magnetic resonance imaging study . NeuroImage , 19 (2), 210-225. 

Jiang, Y. (2004). Resolving dual-task interference: an fMRI study. Neuroimage, 22 (2), 748-754.

Jiang, Y., Saxe, R., & Kanwisher, N . (2004 ). Functional magnetic resonance imaging provides new constraints on theories of the psychological refractory period. Psychological Science, 15 (6), 390-396.

Just, M. A., Carpenter, P. A., Keller, T. A., Emery, L., Zajac, H., & Thulborn, K. R . (2001). Interdependence of nonoverlapping cortical systems in dual cognitive tasks. NeuroImage, 14 (2), 417-426. 

Kahneman, D . (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Karlin, L., & Kestenbaum, R . (1968 ). Effects of number of alternatives on the psychological refractory period . Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology , 20, 167-178.

Kerns, J. G., Cohen, J. D., MacDonald, A. W., Cho, R. Y., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science, 303 (5660), 1023-1026.

Klingberg, T. Concurrent performance of two working memory tasks: potential mechanisms of interference. Cerebral Cortex, 8 (7) , 593-601.

Koechlin, E., Basso, G., Pietrini, P., Panzer, S., & Grafman, J . (1999). The role of the anterior prefrontal cortex in human cognition. Nature, 399 (6732), 148-151.

Koechlin, E., Ody, C., & Kouneiher, F. (2003). The architecture of cognitive control in the human prefrontal cortex. Science , 304, 1181-1185.

Konishi, S., Nakajima, K., Uchida, I., Kameyama, M., Nakahara, K., Sekihara, K., & Miyashita, Y. (1998). Transient activation of inferior prefrontal cortex during cognitive set shifting. Nature Neuroscience, 1 (1), 80-84. 

Kubota, K. & Niki, H. (1971). Prefrontal cortical unit activity and delayed alternation performance in monkeys. Journal of Neurophysiology, 34, 337-347.

Kwong, K. K., Belliveau, J. W., Chesler, D. A., Goldberg, I. E., Weisskoff, R. M., Poncelet, B. P., Kennedy, D.N., Hoppel, B.E., Cohen, M.S., Turner, R., et al. (1992). Dynamic magnetic resonance imaging of human brain activity during primary sensory stimulation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 89 (12), 5675-5679.

Lee, L., Harrison, L. M., & Mechelli, A. (2003). A report of the functional connectivity workshop, Duesseldorf 2002. NeuroImage, 19 (2), 457-465.

Lie, C., Specht, K., Marshall, J. C., & Fink, G. R . (2006). Using fMRI to decompose the neural processes underlying the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test . NeuroImage, 30 (3), 1038-1049. 

Liu, X., Banich, M. T., Jacobson, B. L., & Tanabe, J. L. (2004). Common and distinct neural substrates of attentional control in an integrated Simon and spatial Stroop task as assessed by event-related fMRI. NeuroImage, 22 (3), 1097-1106. 

Logan, G.D., & Gordon, R.D . (2001 ). Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations. Psychological Review, 108, 393-434.

Luria, R., & Meiran, N. (2003). Online order control in the psychological refractory period paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29 (3), 556-574.

  Marois, R., & Ivanoff, J. (2005). Capacity limits of information processing in the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9 (6), 296-305.

McCann, R., & Johnston, J.C. (1992). Locus of the single-channel bottleneck in dual-task interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18 (2), 471-484.

Miezin, F. M., Maccotta, L., Ollinger, J. M., Petersen, S. E., & Buckner, R. L . (2000). Characterizing the hemodynamic response: effects of presentation rate, sampling procedure, and the possibility of ordering brain activity based on relative timing. NeuroImage, 11 (6), 735-759.

Miller, E.K., & Cohen, J.D . (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 167-202.

Miller, E.K., & Desimone, R . (1994). Parallel neuronal mechanisms for short-term memory. Science, 263, 520-522.

Miller, E.K., Erickson, C.A., & Desimone, R. (1996). Neural mechanisms of visual working memory in prefrontal cortex in the macaque . Journal of Neuroscience, 16, 5154-5167.

Meyer, D.E., & Kieras, D.E . (1997). A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: Part1. Basic mechanisms. Psychological Review,   104 (1), 3-65.

Monchi, O., Petrides, M., Petre, V., Worsley, K., & Dagher, A . (2001). Wisconsin Card Sorting revisited: distinct neural circuits participating in different stages of the task identified by event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of neuroscience, 21 (19), 7733-7741. 

Muhammad, R., Wallis, J.D., & Miller, E.K . (2006). A comparison of abstract rules in the prefrontal cortex, premotor cortex, inferior temporal cortex, and striatum. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 61 (12), 974,-989.

Nieto-Castanon, A., Ghosh, S. S., Tourville, J. A., & Guenther, F. H. (2003). Region of interest based analysis of functional imaging data. NeuroImage, 19 (4), 1303-1316. 

Norman, D.A., & Shallice, T . (1986). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior . In Davidson, R.J., Schwartz, G.E., & Shapiro, D. (Eds.). Consciousness and self-regulation (Vol. 4, 99). New York: Plenum, p 1- 18.

Ogawa, S., Lee, T. M., Kay, A. R., & Tank, D. W. (1990). Brain magnetic resonance imaging with contrast dependent on blood oxygenation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 87 (24), 9868-9872.

Ogawa, S., Tank, D. W., Menon, R., Ellermann, J. M., Kim, S. G., Merkle, H., & Uqurbil, K. (1992 ). Intrinsic signal changes accompanying sensory stimulation: functional brain mapping with magnetic resonance imaging. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences , 89 (13), 5951-5955. 

Pashler, H. (1994). Dual-task interference in simple tasks: Data and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 116 , 220-244.

Passingham, R.E. (1993). The frontal lobes and voluntary action. Oxford: Psychology Series, Oxford University Press.

Petrides, M. (2000). The role of the mid-dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in working memory. Experimental Brain Research, 133 (1), 44-54.

Petrides, M., & Pandya, D.N. (1999). Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: comparative cytoarchitectonic analysis in the human and the macaque brain and corticocortical connection patterns. European Journal of Neuroscience, 11, 1011-1036.

Pollmann, S. (2001). Switching between dimensions, locations, and responses: the role of the left frontopolar cortex. NeuroImage, 14 (1), 118-124.

Ramnani, N. & Owen, A.M. (2004). Anterior prefrontal cortex: Insights into function from anatomy and neuroimaging . Nature Reviews Neuroscience , 5, 184-194.

Ridderinkhof, K.R., Ullsperger, M., Crone, E.A., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2004). The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control . Science , 306, 443-447.

Sakai, K., & Passingham, R. E. (2003). Prefrontal interactions reflect future task operations. Nature Neuroscience, 6 (1), 75-81.

Saxe, R., Brett, M., & Kanwisher, N. (2006). Divide and conquer: a defense of functional localizers. NeuroImage, 30 (4), 1088-1099.

Schubert, T. (1999). Processing differences between simple and choice reaction affect bottleneck localization in overlapping tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , 25, 408-425.

Schubert, T., von Cramon, D.Y., Niendorf, T., Pollmann, S., & Bublak, P . (1998). Cortical areas and the control of self-determined finger movements: an fMRI study. Neuroreport , 9 (14), 3171-3176.

Schubert, T., & Szameitat, A.J . (2003). Functional neuroanatomy of interference in overlapping dual tasks: an fMRI study. Cognitive Brain Research, 17 (3), 733-746.

Schumacher, E. H., Seymour, T. L., Glass, J. M., Fencsik, D. E., Lauber, E. J., Kieras, D. E., & Meyer, D.E. (2001). Virtually perfect time sharing in dual-task performance: uncorking the central cognitive bottleneck . Psychological Science, 12 (2), 101-108.

Schumacher, E. H., & D'Esposito, M. (2002). Neural implementation of response selection in humans as revealed by localized effects of stimulus-response compatibility on brain activation . Human Brain Mapping, 17 (3), 193-201.

Schumacher, E. H., Elston, P. A., & D'Esposito, M. (2003). Neural evidence for representation-specific response selection. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15 (8), 1111-1121.

Shima, K., Isoda, M., Mushiake, H., & Tanji, J. (2007). Categorization of behavioural sequences in the prefrontal cortex. Nature, 445 (7125), 315-318.

Sigman, M., & Dehaene, S. (2006). Dynamics of the central bottleneck: dual-task and task uncertainty . PLOS Biology, 4 (7): e 220.

Smith, E. E., Geva, A., Jonides, J., Miller, A., Reuter-Lorenz, P., & Koeppe, R. A. (2001). The neural basis of task-switching in working memory: effects of performance and aging. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science , 98 (4), 2095-2100.

Stuss, D.T., Levine, B., Alexander, M.P., Hong, J., Palumo, C., Hamer, L., Murphy, K.J., & Izukawa, D. (2000). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in patients with frontal and posterior brain damage: effects of lesion location and test structure on separable cognitive processes. Neuropsychologia, 38 (4), 388-402.

Stuss, D.T., Floden, D., Alexander, M.P., Levine, B., & Katz, D. (2001). Stroop performance in focal lesion patients: dissociation of processes and frontal lobe lesion location. Neuropsychologia, 39 (8), 771-786.

Sternberg, S . (1969). The discovery of processing stages: extensions of Donders’ method. Acta Psycholgica , 30 , 276-315.

Swainson, R., Cunnington, R., Jackson, G. M., Rorden, C., Peters, A. M., Morris, P. G., & Jackson, S. (2003). Cognitive control mechanisms revealed by ERP and fMRI: evidence from repeated task-switching. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15 (6), 785-799.

Szameitat, A. J., Schubert, T., Mueller, K., & von Cramon, D.Y. (2002). Localization of executive functions in dual-task performance with fMRI. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14 (8), 1184-1199.

Szameitat, A.J., Lepsien, J., von Cramon, D.Y., Sterr, A., & Schubert, T. (2006). Task-order coordination in dual-task performance and the lateral prefrontal cortex. An event-related fMRI study. Psychological Research, 70 (6), 541-552.

Telford, C.W. (1931). The refractory phase of voluntary and associative reponses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 14, 1-36.

Wallis, J.D., Anderson, K.C., & Miller, E.K. (2001 ). Single neurons in prefrontal cortex encode abstract rules . Nature, 411 (6840), 953-956.

Welford, A.T . (1952). The ’psychological refractory period’ and the timing of high speed performance: a review and a theory. British Journal of Psychology , 43 , 2-9.

Zysset, S., Müller, K., Lohmann, G., & von Cramon , D. Y. (2001). Color-word matching stroop task: separating interference and response conflict . NeuroImage, 13 (1), 29-36. 

Publications submitted

Original articles

Stelzel, C., Schumacher, E.H., Schubert, T., & D’Esposito, M. (2006). The neural effect of stimulus-response modality compatibility on dual-task performance: an fMRI study. Psychological Research, 70(6), 514-525.

Stelzel, C., Kraft, A., Brandt, S.A., & Schubert, T. (in press). Dissociable neural effects of task order control and task set maintenance during dual-task processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(4), 613-628.

Stelzel, C., Brandt, S.A., & Schubert, T. Neural mechanisms of attentional task setting in dual tasks. Submitted to Cerebral Cortex, October 2007.


© Die inhaltliche Zusammenstellung und Aufmachung dieser Publikation sowie die elektronische Verarbeitung sind urheberrechtlich geschützt. Jede Verwertung, die nicht ausdrücklich vom Urheberrechtsgesetz zugelassen ist, bedarf der vorherigen Zustimmung. Das gilt insbesondere für die Vervielfältigung, die Bearbeitung und Einspeicherung und Verarbeitung in elektronische Systeme.
DiML DTD Version 4.0Zertifizierter Dokumentenserver
der Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin
HTML generated:
12.06.2008